« Go back to issue 46(1) summary

The complex interrelationship between dissociation and anomalous sleep experiences

Everton de Oliveira Maraldi1

1 Pontifical Catholic University of São Paulo, São Paulo, SP, Brazil.

Received: 3/20/2018 – Accepted: 11/7/2018

DOI: 10.1590/0101-60830000000187

Address for correspondence: Everton de Oliveira Maraldi. Post-Graduate Program on Religious Studies, Pontifical Catholic University of São Paulo. Ministro Godoy Street, 969, 4º andar, Sala 4E-09 – 05015-901 – São Paulo, SP, Brazil. Telephone: (11) 3670-8529. E-mail: [email protected]

Maraldi EO / Arch Clin Psychiatry. 2019;46(1):25-6

Dear editor,

Dissociative experiences are common in the general population, with about one-third of individuals reporting at least one dissociative symptom1,2. The etiology of dissociation (especially of its more pathological expressions) has been primarily considered in terms of childhood trauma, but critiques of the trauma model also point the need of considering cognitive, psychopathological and sociocultural factors other than trauma in a comprehensive model of dissociative experiences3. Of particular interest here is the growing body of research uncovering the complex interrelationship between dissociation and anomalous sleep experiences such as recurrent nightmares, vivid and bizarre dreams, sleep paralysis, hypnagogic/hypnopompic imagery, and narcolepsy symptoms4,5.

This topic had already been discussed by some of the pioneers of the study of dissociation, such as Pierre Janet (1859-1947) and Morton Prince (1854-1929), but only recently did the subject gain research attention6. The dream has long been considered an interesting paradigm for dissociative experiences such as absorption/imaginative involvement and multiple identity states. Hilgard7, for example, defined the dream as a dissociative phenomenon per se. A significant body of evidence gives some credence to Hilgard’s hypothesis. Yu8, for example, found that the intensity of a person’s dreams is a good predictor of dissociative tendencies and conversion symptoms. Highly dissociative individuals, including patients diagnosed with dissociative disorders, usually report more vivid dreams and other unusual sleep experiences9. Dissociative experiences may also be confused with episodes of somnambulism, occurring in circumstances during which individuals remain physiologically awake10. However, the prevalence of dissociation during anomalous sleep experiences is largely unknown, either for general or clinical populations.

In a longitudinal experiment, Giesbrecht et al.11 assessed every six hours the dissociative experiences of 25 healthy volunteers (deprived of sleep for one night). The researchers found that, although dissociative symptoms remained stable during the day, they significantly augmented during the night. The results remained statistically significant even after controlling for a series of confounders. Similarly, in a study by Denis et al.12, the frequency of dissociative experiences reported during the day was found to be related to both sleep paralysis frequency and the frequency/intensity of hallucinations experienced during sleep paralysis episodes.

It was hypothesized that disturbances of the sleep-wake cycle may cause or even maintain dissociative phenomena13. In a study with insomniac patients, Van Der Kloet et al.13 assessed dissociative symptoms and EEG sleep parameters and found that lengthening of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep predicts dissociation. In its turn, insomniac patients revealed high dissociation scores. Based on the hypothesis that sleep deprivation and other disturbances in the sleep-wake cycle would lead to dissociative intrusions of sleep phenomena in the waking state, Van der Kloet et al.14 demonstrated that the improvement in sleep normalization and narcolepsy symptoms accompanied a significant decrease in the level of dissociation and general psychopathology in a clinical sample. It seems thus that sleep hygiene may contribute to the treatment or prevention of dissociative symptoms.

The above therapeutic findings may suggest, however, that dissociation is not related specifically to anomalous sleep experiences, but to sleep quality more generally. It is known that a number of the sleep experiences mentioned (e.g. sleep paralysis) has been consistently linked to general sleep quality and insomnia symptoms. Koffel and Watson15, however, cite studies suggesting that dissociation shares a specific relationship with anomalous sleep experiences, as opposed to insomnia.

Dissociation also showed to be related to a series of non-pathological anomalous sleep experiences such as lucid dreaming16 and out-of-body experiences17. Based on the existing evidence, Cardeña and Alvarado18 concluded that dissociation is one of the best predictors (along with hypnotic susceptibility and fantasy proneness) of out-of-body experiences. On the other hand, other studies have found that dissociative experiences correlate with nightmares and waking dreams, but do not correlate with lucid dreaming19, thereby suggesting that dissociation is only related to those sleep experiences that are difficult to control. More investigations are needed to understand the precise role of dissociation on lucid dreaming.

The relationship between anomalous sleep experiences and dissociation is a relatively unexplored research field pointing to a series of explanatory or etiological pathways to dissociative experiences beyond childhood trauma. In this sense, it may have implications beyond clinical practice. Anomalous sleep experiences such as sleep paralysis and bizarre dreams have given rise to many folkloric and paranormal beliefs, such as reports of alien abductions and attacks of monstrous creatures during the night20. This is a topic of potential relevance for the understanding of how similar beliefs about sleep-related phenomena emerged in different cultures.

The evidence reviewed above suggests that researchers have much to gain from the investigation of anomalous sleep experiences, either in terms of the development of new hypotheses on the nature of dissociation or regarding the treatment of dissociative symptoms.

Funding

This work was funded by São Paulo Research Foundation (Fapesp) under the grant number #2015/05255-2.

1. Ross CA, Joshi S, Currie R. Dissociative experiences in the general population. Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147(11):1547-52.

2. Seedat S, Stein MB, Forde DR. Prevalence of dissociative experiences in a community sample: relationship to gender, ethnicity, and substance use. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2003;191(2):115-20.

3. Lynn SJ, Lilienfeld, SO, Merckelbach H, Giesbrecht T, McNally RJ, Loftus EF, et al. The trauma model of dissociation: inconvenient truths and stubborn fictions. Comments on Dalenberg et al. (2012). Psychol Bull. 2014;140(3):896-910.

4. Giesbrecht T, Merckelbach H. Subjective sleep experiences are related to dissociation. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;37(7):1341-5.

5. Giesbrecht T, Merckelbach H. Dreaming to reduce fantasy: fantasy proneness, dissociation and subjective sleep experiences. Pers Individ Dif. 2006;41(4):697-706.

6. Barret D. The relationship of dissociative conditions to sleep and dreaming. In: Krippner S, Powers SM, editors. Broken images, broken selves: dissociative narratives in clinical practice. Washington: Brunner/Mazel; 1997. p. 216-29.

7. Hilgard ER. The new interpretation of dreams in relation to neo-dissociation theory. J Ment Imagery. 1992;16(1-2):125-30.

8. Yu CKC. Dream intensity profile as an indicator of the hysterical tendencies to dissociation and conversion. Dreaming. 2010;20(3):184-97.

9. Van der Kloet D, Merckelbach H, Giesbrecht T, Lynn SJ. Fragmented sleep, fragmented mind: the role of sleep in dissociative symptoms. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2012;7(2):159-75.

10. Fleming J. Dissociative episodes presenting as somnambulism: a case report. Sleep Res. 1987;16:263.

11. Giesbrecht T, Smeets T, Leppink J, Jelicic M, Merckelbach H. Acute dissociation after 1 night of sleep loss. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116(3):599-606.

12. Denis D, French CC, Gregory AM. A systematic review of variables associated with sleep paralysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2018;38:141-57.

13. Van der Kloet D, Giesbrecht T, Franck E, Van Gastel A, De Volder I, Van Den Eede F, et al. Dissociative symptoms and sleep parameters – an all-night polysomnography study in patients with insomnia. Compr Psychiatry. 2013;54(6):658-64.

14. Van der Kloet D, Giesbrecht T, Lynn SJ, Merckelbach H, de Zutter A. Sleep normalization and decrease in dissociative experiences: evaluation in an inpatient sample. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121(1):140-50.

15. Koffel E, Watson D. Unusual sleep experiences, dissociation, and schizotypy: evidence for a common domain. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009:29(6):548-59.

16. Denis D, Poerio GL. Terror and bliss? Commonalities and distinctions between sleep paralysis, lucid dreaming, and their associations with waking life experiences. J Sleep Res. 2017:26(1):38-47.

17. Irwin HJ. The disembodied self: an empirical study of dissociation and the out-of- body experience. J Parapsychol. 2000; 64: 261-277.

18. Cardeña E, Alvarado CS. Anomalous self and identity experiences. In: Cardeña E, Lynn SJ, Krippner S, editors. Varieties of anomalous experience: examining the scientific evidence. 2nd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2014. p. 175-212.

19. Voss U, Holzmann R, Tuin I, Hobson JA. Lucid dreaming: a state of consciousness with features of both waking and non-lucid dreaming. Sleep. 2009;32(9):1191-200.

20. Hufford DJ. The terror that comes in the night: an experience-centred study of supernatural assault traditions. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press; 1982.